Home / Health / News Tools: Save | Print | E-mail | Most Read | Comment
Sleep selectively preserves emotional memories
Adjust font size:

A period of slumber helps the brain distinguish core emotions from background details

As poets, songwriters and authors have described, our memories range from misty water-colored recollections to vividly detailed images of the times of our lives.

Now, a study led by researchers at Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center (BIDMC) and Boston College offers new insights into the specific components of emotional memories, suggesting that sleep plays a key role in determining what we remember – and what we forget.

Reported in the August 2008 issue of the journal Psychological Science, the findings show that a period of slumber helps the brain to selectively preserve and enhance those aspects of a memory that are of greatest emotional resonance, while at the same time diminishing the memory's neutral background details.

"This tells us that sleep's role in emotional memory preservation is more than just mechanistic," says the study's first author Jessica Payne, PhD, a Harvard University research fellow in the Division of Psychiatry at BIDMC. "In order to preserve what it deems most important, the brain makes a tradeoff, strengthening the memory's emotional core and obscuring its neutral background."

Previous studies have established the key role that sleep plays in procedural memory, demonstrating that the consolidation of procedural skills (such as typing or playing the piano) is greatly enhanced following a period of sleep.

But sleep's importance in the development of episodic memories – in particular, those with emotional resonance– has been less clear.

"Emotional memories usually contain highly charged elements – for example, the car that sideswiped us on the ride home – along with other elements that are only tangentially related to the emotion, such as the name of the street we were traveling on or what store we'd just passed," explains study author Elizabeth Kensinger, PhD, an Assistant Professor in the College of Arts and Sciences at Boston College. "We were interested in examining whether sleep would affect memory for all of these elements equally, or whether sleep might allow some of the event features to decay at a faster rate than others."

The authors tested 88 college students. Study participants were shown scenes that depicted either neutral subjects on a neutral background (a car parked on a street in front of shops) or negatively arousing subjects on a neutral background (a badly crashed car parked on a similar street). The participants were then tested separately on their memories of both the central objects in the pictures and the backgrounds in the scenes. In this way, memory could be compared for the emotional aspects of a scene (the crashed car) versus the non-emotional aspects of the scene (the street on which the car had crashed.)

Subjects were divided into three groups. The first group underwent memory testing after 12 hours spent awake during the daytime; the second group was tested after 12 nighttime hours, including their normal period of nighttime sleep; and the third baseline group was tested 30 minutes after viewing the images, in either the morning or evening.

"Our results revealed that the study subjects who stayed awake all day largely forgot the entire negative scene [they had seen], with their memories of both the central objects and the backgrounds decaying at similar rates," says Payne. But, she adds, among the individuals who were tested after a period of sleep, memory recall for the central negative objects (i.e. the smashed car) was preserved in detail.

"After an evening of sleep, the subjects remembered the emotional items [smashed car] as accurately as the subjects whose memories had been tested only 30 minutes after looking at the scenes," explains Kensinger. "By contrast, sleep did little to preserve memory for the backgrounds [i.e. street scenes] and so memory for those elements reached a comparably low level after a night of sleep as it did after a day spent awake."

"This is consistent with the possibility that the individual components of emotional memory become 'unbound' during sleep," adds Payne, explaining that "unbinding" enables the sleeping brain to selectively preserve only that information which it calculates to be most salient and worthy of remembering. A real-world example of this tradeoff, she adds, is the "weapon focus effect" in which crime victims vividly remember an assailant's weapon, but have little memory for other important aspects of the crime scene. Traumatic memories, such as the flashbacks experienced among individuals with post-traumatic stress disorder, can demonstrate similar disparities, with some aspects of an experience seemingly engraved in memory while other details are erased.

"Sleep is a smart, sophisticated process," adds Payne. "You might say that sleep is actually working at night to decide what memories to hold on to and what to let go of."

(Agencies August 14, 2008)

Tools: Save | Print | E-mail | Most Read
Comment
Pet Name
Anonymous
China Archives
Related >>
Most Viewed >>
- Fit and fat: Study shows it's possible
- Difference between married, unmarried narrowed
- UNICEF rallies youths to raise HIV/AIDS awareness
- Potatoes may hold key to Alzheimer's treatment
- Easy does it - Don't sit too long on your butt
主站蜘蛛池模板: 中文字幕专区高清在线观看| 亚洲愉拍一区二区三区| 视频一区二区三区在线观看| 国产精品拍拍拍| aa级黄色毛片| 好紧好爽欲yy18p| 中文字幕人妻无码一夲道| 日韩无人区电影| 亚洲一卡2卡3卡4卡国产网站| 高清破外女出血视频| 国产精品成人久久久久| gogo全球高清大胆亚洲| 成人h在线播放| 久久99精品久久久久久青青日本 | 亚洲欧美日韩精品久久亚洲区| 韩国一级做a爱性色毛片| 国产福利精品一区二区| 3d玉蒲团之极乐宝鉴| 国精产品wnw2544a| mm131美女做爽爽爱视频| 影音先锋成人资源| 中文字幕亚洲激情| 日本xxxx裸体bbbb| 久久久婷婷五月亚洲97号色| 日韩免费在线观看视频| 九一制片厂果冻传媒56| 极品丝袜老师h系列全文| 亚洲伊人久久精品| 欧美亚洲第一区| 亚洲国产精品久久久久秋霞影院| 穆天阳吃饭还在顶是哪一章节| 国产在线一区二区三区av| 抽搐一进一出gif免费视频| 欧美综合区自拍亚洲综合图区| 四虎精品视频在线永久免费观看| 香蕉在线精品视频在线观看2| 小次郎收藏最新地址| 亚洲免费视频播放| 欧美日本高清视频在线观看| 亚洲第一视频网站| 美美女高清毛片视频免费观看|